The Community for Technology Leaders
RSS Icon
Subscribe
Issue No.03 - July-Sept. (2013 vol.6)
pp: 352-362
Inwook Hwang , Dept. of Comput. Sci. & Eng., Pohang Univ. of Sci. & Technol., Pohang, South Korea
Jongman Seo , Dept. of Comput. Sci. & Eng., Pohang Univ. of Sci. & Technol., Pohang, South Korea
Myongchan Kim , Dept. of Comput. Sci. & Eng., Pohang Univ. of Sci. & Technol., Pohang, South Korea
Seungmoon Choi , Dept. of Comput. Sci. & Eng., Pohang Univ. of Sci. & Technol., Pohang, South Korea
ABSTRACT
Vibrotactile rendering is an emerging interaction method for information transmission in mobile devices, replacing or supplementing visual and auditory displays. To design effective vibrotactile actuators or display algorithms, an understanding of the perceived intensity (strength) of their vibrations is essential. This paper aims to build a robust model for the perceived intensities of mobile device vibrations, which can be immediately used by engineers and application designers. To this end, we carried out two psychophysical experiments using absolute magnitude estimation procedures. In Experiment I, we investigated the effects of vibration direction and device weight on the perceived intensity of mobile device vibrations. The vibration directions tested (height, width, and depth), and the device weights (90-130 g) were determined considering those of contemporary mobile devices. Only the vibration direction was found to be a statistically significant factor, showing the highest perceived intensities along the height direction of a mobile device. In Experiment II, we measured the perceived intensities of vibrations with various amplitudes and frequencies along the three vibration directions. Then, for each direction, a psychophysical magnitude function and equal sensation contours were constructed based on Stevens' power law, which clearly visualize the consequences of vibration parameter changes on the resulting perceptual strength. In addition, we found a monotonic relationship between the physical power of vibration absorbed by the hand and the resulting perceived intensity. This suggests that the former, which is greatly easier to acquire in practice, is a reliable predictor of the latter. We expect that the results of this study can provide immediate knowledge about the perceptual strength of vibrations that engineers and applications developers will find useful.
INDEX TERMS
Vibrations, Mobile handsets, Actuators, Vibration measurement, Standards, Force, Frequency measurement,skin-absorbed power, Perceived intensity, mobile device, vibration, direction, weight
CITATION
Inwook Hwang, Jongman Seo, Myongchan Kim, Seungmoon Choi, "Vibrotactile Perceived Intensity for Mobile Devices as a Function of Direction, Amplitude, and Frequency", IEEE Transactions on Haptics, vol.6, no. 3, pp. 352-362, July-Sept. 2013, doi:10.1109/TOH.2013.2
REFERENCES
[1] R.W. Cholewiak, "The Design of Vibrotatile Transducers," Tactile Aids for the Hearing Impaired, I.R. Summers, ed., pp. 57-82, Whurr Publishers, 1992.
[2] L.A. Jones and S.J. Lederman, Human Hand Function. Oxford Univ. Press, 2006.
[3] R.T. Verrillo and G.A. Gescheider, "Perception via the Sense of Touch," Tactile Aids for the Hearing Impaired, I.R. Summers, ed., pp. 1-36, Whurr Publishers, 1992.
[4] R.T. Verrillo, "Measurement of Vibrotactile Sensation Magnitude," Ratio Scaling of Psychological Magnitude: In Honor of the Memory of S. S. Stevens, S.J. Bolanowski and G.A. Gescheider, eds., pp. 260-275, Lawrence Erlbaum, 1991.
[5] J. Ryu, J. Jung, G. Park, and S. Choi, "Psychophysical Model for Vibrotactile Rendering in Mobile Devices," Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, vol. 19, no. 4, pp. 364-387, 2010.
[6] G.A. Gescheider, Psychophysics: The Fundamentals, third ed. Lawrence Erlbaum, 1997.
[7] M. Morioka and M.J. Griffin, "Magnitude-Dependence of Equivalent Comfort Contours for Fore-and-Aft, Lateral and Vertical Hand-Transmitted Vibration," J. Sound and Vibration, vol. 295, nos. 3-5, pp. 633-648, 2006.
[8] S.S. Stevens, "Tactile Vibration: Dynamics of Sensory Intensity," J. Experimental Psychology, vol. 57, no. 4, pp. 210-218, 1959.
[9] J. Ryu, C.-W. Lee, and S. Choi, "Improving Vibrotactile Pattern Identification for Mobile Devices Using Perceptually Transparent Rendering," Proc. Int'l Conf. Human Computer Interaction with Mobile Devices and Services, pp. 257-260, 2010.
[10] T. Miwa, "Evaluation Methods for Vibration Effect. Part 3. Measurements of Threshold and Equal Sensation Contours on Hand for Vertical and Horizontal Sinusoidal Vibrations," Industrial Health, vol. 5, pp. 213-220, 1967.
[11] D.D. Reynolds, K.G. Standlee, and E.N. Angevine, "Hand-Arm Vibration, Part III: Subjective Response Characteristics of Individuals to Hand-Induced Vibration," J. Sound and Vibration, vol. 51, no. 2, pp. 267-282, 1977.
[12] H. Yao, D. Grant, and M. Cruz, "Perceived Vibration Strength in Mobile Devices: The Effect of Weight and Frequency," IEEE Trans. Haptics, vol. 3, no. 1, pp. 56-62, Jan.-Mar. 2010.
[13] I. Hwang and S. Choi, "Effect of Mechanical Ground on the Vibrotactile Perceived Intensity of a Handheld Object," Proc. Int'l Conf. Haptics: Perception, Devices, Mobility, and Comm., pp. 61-66, 2012.
[14] L.J. Post, I.C. Zompa, and C.E. Chapman, "Perception of Vibrotactile Stimuli during Motor Activity in Human Subjects," Experimental Brain Research, vol. 100, no. 1, pp. 107-120, 1994.
[15] A.J. Brisben, S.S. Hsiao, and K.O. Johnson, "Detection of Vibration Transmitted through an Object Grasped in the Hand," J. Neurophysiology, vol. 81, no. 4, pp. 1548-1558, 1999.
[16] S.H. Han, M. Song, and J. Kwahk, "A Systematic Method for Analyzing Magnitude Estimation Data," Int'l J. Industrial Ergonomics, vol. 23, pp. 513-524, 1999.
[17] M. Morioka and M.J. Griffin, "Thresholds for the Perception of Hand-Transmitted Vibration: Dependence on Contact Area and Contact Location," Somatosensory and Motor Research, vol. 22, no. 4, pp. 281-297, 2005.
[18] M. Morioka and M.J. Griffin, "Independent Responses of Pacinian and Non-Pacinian Systems with Hand-Transmitted Vibration Detected from Masked Thresholds," Somatosensory and Motor Research, vol. 22, nos. 1/2, pp. 69-84, 2005.
[19] S.J. BolanowskiJr., G.A. Gescheider, R.T. Verrillo, and C.M. Checkosky, "Four Channels Mediate the Mechanical Aspects of Touch," J. Acoustical Soc. of Am., vol. 84, pp. 1680-1694, 1988.
[20] R.T. Verrillo and A.J. Capraro, "Effect of Stimulus Frequency on Subjective Vibrotactile Magnitude Functions," Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, vol. 17, no. 1, pp. 91-96, 1975.
[21] G.A. Gescheider and B.A. Hughson, "Stimulus Context and Absolute Magnitude Estimation: A Study of Individual Differences," Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, vol. 50, no. 1, pp. 45-57, 1991.
[22] J.C. Makous, R.M. Friedman, and C.J. VierckJr., "A Critical Band Filter in Touch," J. Neuroscience, vol. 15, no. 4, pp. 2808-2818, 1995.
[23] S.J. Bensmaïa, M. Hollins, and J. Yau, "Vibrotactile Intensity and Frequency Information in the Pacinian System: A Psychophysical Model," Perception and Psychophysics, vol. 67, no. 5, pp. 828-841, 2005.
[24] R. Lundström, "Local Vibrations-Mechanical Impedance of the Human Hand's Glabrous Skin," J. Biomechanics, vol. 17, no. 2, pp. 137-144, 1984.
[25] R.G. Dong, D.E. Welcome, T.W. McDowell, J.Z. Wu, and A.W. Schopper, "Frequency Weighting Derived from Power Absorption of Fingers-Hand-Arm System under $z_h$ -Axis Vibration," J. Biomechanics, vol. 39, no. 12, pp. 2311-2324, 2006.
[26] S. Kihlberg, "Biodynamic Response of the Hand-Arm System to Vibration from an Impact Hammer and a Grinder," Int'l J. Industrial Ergonomics, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 1-8, 1995.
35 ms
(Ver 2.0)

Marketing Automation Platform Marketing Automation Tool