This Article 
   
 Share 
   
 Bibliographic References 
   
 Add to: 
 
Digg
Furl
Spurl
Blink
Simpy
Google
Del.icio.us
Y!MyWeb
 
 Search 
   
Benefitting InfoVis with Visual Difficulties
Dec. 2011 (vol. 17 no. 12)
pp. 2213-2222
Jessica Hullman, University of Michigan School of Information
Eytan Adar, University of Michigan School of Information
Priti Shah, University of Michigan
Many well-cited theories for visualization design state that a visual representation should be optimized for quick and immediate interpretation by a user. Distracting elements like decorative "chartjunk" or extraneous information are avoided so as not to slow comprehension. Yet several recent studies in visualization research provide evidence that non-efficient visual elements may benefit comprehension and recall on the part of users. Similarly, findings from studies related to learning from visual displays in various subfields of psychology suggest that introducing cognitive difficulties to visualization interaction can improve a user's understanding of important information. In this paper, we synthesize empirical results from cross-disciplinary research on visual information representations, providing a counterpoint to efficiency-based design theory with guidelines that describe how visual difficulties can be introduced to benefit comprehension and recall. We identify conditions under which the application of visual difficulties is appropriate based on underlying factors in visualization interaction like active processing and engagement. We characterize effective graph design as a trade-off between efficiency and learning difficulties in order to provide Information Visualization (InfoVis) researchers and practitioners with a framework for organizing explorations of graphs for which comprehension and recall are crucial. We identify implications of this view for the design and evaluation of information visualizations.

[1] C. Ahlberg and E. Wistrand, IVEE: an Information Visualization and Exploration Environment, in Proc. of Vis.'95, 1995.
[2] A.L. Alter, D.M. Oppenheimer, N. Epley, and R.N. Eyre, Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning, J. of Exp. Psych.-Gen., vol. 136, no. 4, 2007.
[3] A. L. Alter and D. M. Oppenheimer, Uniting the Tribes of Fluency to Form a Metacognitive Nation, Personality and Social Psychology Review, vol. 13, no. 3, 2009.
[4] D. Archambault, H. C. Purchase, and B. Pinaud, Animation, Small Multiples, and the Effect of Mental Map Preservation in Dynamic Graphs, IEEE TVCG, vol. 17, no. 4, 2011.
[5] S. Bateman, R. L. Mandryk, C. Gutwin, A. Genest, D. McDine, and C. Brooks, Useful junk?: the effects of visual embellishment on comprehension and memorability of charts, CHI, 2010.
[6] G. Beenen et al., Using social psychology to motivate contributions to online communities, in CSCW, 2004.
[7] E. Bertini, A. Perer, C. Plaisant, and G. Santucci, BELIV'08: Beyond time and errors novel evaluation methods for information visualization (Workshop), CHI, 2008.
[8] R. A. Bjork, Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe et al. (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1994.
[9] E.L. Bjork & R.A. Bjork, Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In M. A. Gernsbacher et al. (Eds.), , Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society. New York: Worth Publishers, 2011.
[10] A. J. Blasio and A. M. Bisantz, A comparison of the effects of data-ink ratio on performance with dynamic displays in a monitoring task, Int.'l J. of Indust. Ergonomics, vol. 30, no. 2, 2002.
[11] M. Burton, D. P. Wickham, L. Phelps, K. Spain, J. Crews, and N. Rich, Secondary navigation in software wizards, in CHI '99 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems, New York, NY, USA, 1999, p. 294–295.
[12] J. T. Cacioppo, R. E. Petty, J. A. Feinstein, and W. B. G. Jarvis, Dispositional Differences in Cognitive Motivation: The Life and Times of Individuals Varying in Need for Cognition, Psych. Bulletin, vol. 119, no. 2, 1996.
[13] P. A. Carpenter and P. Shah, A Model of the Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Graph Comprehension, J. of Exp. Psych.: Appl., vol. 4, no. 2, 1998.
[14] C.M. Carswell, S. Frankenberger, and D. Bernhard, Graphing in depth: perspectives on the use of three-dimensional graphs to represent lower-dimensional data, Behav. & Info. Tech., vol. 10, no. 6, 1991.
[15] C.M. Carswell, Choosing specifiers: an evaluation of the basic tasks model of graphical perception, Human Factors, vol. 34, no. 5, pp. 535-554, Oct. 1992.
[16] S. Casner and J. H. Larkin, Cognitive Efficiency Considerations for Good Graphic Design. Technical Report, 1989.
[17] R. Chang, C. Ziemkiewicz, R. Pyzh, and J. Kielman, Learning-Based Evaluation of Visual Analytic Systems. BELIV '08 (CHI), 2008.
[18] M.T.H. Chi, M. Bassok, M.W. Lewis, P. Reimann, and R. Glaser, “Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems, Cog. Sci., vol. 13, no. 2, 1989.
[19] M.T.H. Chi, Self-explaining expository texts: The dual processes of generating inferences and repairing mental models. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in Instructional Psychology, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates 2000.
[20] W. S., Cleveland and R. McGill, Graphical Perception: Theory, Experimentation, and Application to the Development of Graphical Methods, J.of the Amer. Stat. Assoc., vol. 79, no. 387, 1984.
[21] J.L. Davenport, D. Klahr & K. Koedinger, The influence of external representations on chemistry problem solving. Presented at the Ann. Meeting of the Psychonomic Soc., 2006.
[22] N. Diakopoulos, F. Kivran-Swaine, and M. Naaman, Playable Data: Characterizing the Design Space of Game-y Infographics, CHI (forthcoming), 2011.
[23] C. Diemand-Yauman, D. M. Oppenheimer, and E. B. Vaughan, Fortune favors the Bold and the Italicized: Effects of disfluency on educational outcomes, Cognition, vol. 118, no. 1, 2011.
[24] ESP game. GWAP.com, 2009. http:/www.espgame.org.
[25] F.L. Fahy, I. P. Riches, and M.W. Brown, Neuronal activity related to visual recognition memory: long-term memory and the encoding of recency and familiarity information in the primate anterior and medial inferior temporal and rhinal cortex, Exp.. Brain Research., vol. 96, no. 3, 1993.
[26] L. Findlater, K McGrenere, and J. Dawson, Ephemeral adaptation: the use of gradual onset to improve menu selection performance, CHI, 2009.
[27] D.J. Gillan & E.H. Richman, Minimalism and the Syntax of Graphs. Human Factors, vol. 36, no. 4, 1994.
[28] D. Hammond, Effectiveness of cigarette warning labels in informing smokers about the risks of smoking: findings from the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Four Country Survey, Tobacco Control, vol. 15, no. 3, 2006.
[29] J. Heer and G. Robertson, “Animated Transitions in Statistical Data Graphics,” IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, vol. 13, no. 6, 2007.
[30] M. Hegarty, Representations in the Mind and in the World: How Cognitive Science Can Inform the Design of Visualizations, Keynote presentation, INFOVIS '10, 2010.
[31] M. Hegarty and D. Waller, Individual Differences in Spatial Abilities. In P. Shah, and A. Miyake (Eds), , The Cambridge handbook of visuospatial thinking. Cambridge University Press, 2005.
[32] M. Hegarty, S. Kriz & C. Cate,, The roles of mental animations and external animations in understanding mechanical systems. Cognition & Instruction, 21, 2003.
[33] .T. Höffler & D. Leutner, Instructional Animation Versus Static Pictures: A Meta- Analysis. Learning and Instruction, vol. 17, 2007.
[34] W. Huang et al.. (Eds.), Measuring effectiveness of graph visualizations: A cognitive load perspective, Information Visualization, vol. 8, no. 3, 2009.
[35] O. Inbar, N. Tractinsky, and J. Meyer, Minimalism in information visualization: attitudes towards maximizing the data-ink ratio, in Proc. of the Euro. Conf. on Cog. Ergonomics, 2007,
[36] R. Kosara, Visualization Criticism – The Missing Link Between Information Visualization and Art, IV '07, 2007.
[37] S. M. Kosslyn, Understanding charts and graphs, Applied Cognitive Psychology, vol. 3, no. 3, 1989.
[38] H. Lam, A Framework of Interaction Costs in Information Visualization, IEEE TVCG, 2008.
[39] H. Lam, E. Bertini, P. Isenberg, C. Plaisant & S. Carpendale, Seven Guiding Scenarios for Information Visualization Evaluation. Technical Report, 2010.
[40] J. H. Larkin, and H. A. Simon, Why a Diagram is (Sometimes) Worth Ten Thousand Words, Cog. Sci., vol. 11, no. 1, 1987.
[41] E. Levy, J. Zacks, B. Tversky, and D. Schiano, Gratuitous graphics? Putting preferences in perspective, in CHI'96, 1996.
[42] G.L. Lohse, The role of working memory on graphical information processing, Behaviour & Information Technology, vol. 16, no. 6, 1997.
[43] G.L. Lohse, A cognitive model for the perception and understanding of graphs, CHI'91, 1991.
[44] Z. Liu and J. T. Stasko, Mental models, visual reasoning and interaction in information visualization: a top-down perspective, IEEE TVCG, vol. 16, no. 6, 2010.
[45] R.E. Mayer, M. Hegarty, S. Mayer, and J. Campbell, When static media promote active learning: annotated illustrations versus narrated animations in multimedia instruction, J. of Exp. Psych.: Appl., vol. 11, no. 4, 2005.
[46] T. Munzner, A Nested Model for Visualization Designand Validation. IEEE TVCG, vol. 15, no. 6, 2009.
[47] H. M. Natter and D. C. Berry, Effects of active information processing on the understanding of risk information, Appl. Cog. Psych., vol. 19, no. 1, 2005.
[48] C. North, Visualization viewpoints: Toward measuring visualization insight. IEEE Comp. Graphics and Applic., vol. 26 no. 3, 2006.
[49] O. Pagliano and D. M. Bajzek, Improving Animation Tutorials by Integrating Simulation, Assessment, and Feedback to Promote Active Learning, in T. Bastiaens & S. Carliner (Eds.), Proc. of World Conf. on E-Learning in Corp., Gov', t, Healthcare, and Higher Educ., 2007.
[50] A. Perer and B. Shneiderman, Systematic yet flexible discovery: guiding domain experts through exploratory data analysis, in Proc. on Intelligent User Interfaces, 2008.
[51] S. Pinker, A theory of graph comprehension. In R. Friedle (Ed.), , Artificial intelligence and the future of testing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1990.
[52] H. Purchase, E. Hoggan, and C. Gorg, How important is the Mental map? an empirical investigation of a dynamic graph layout algorithm. In Graph Drawing, 2006.
[53] G. G. Robertson, S. K. Card, and J. D. Mackinlay, Information visualization using 3D interactive animation, Comm. of the ACM, vol. 36, 1993.
[54] G. Robertson, R. Fernandez, D. Fisher, B. Lee, and J. Stasko, Effectiveness of Animation in Trend Visualization, IEEE TVCG, vol. 14, Nov. 2008.
[55] M. Roy & M.T.H. Chi, The self-explanation principle. In R.E. Mayer (Ed.) , Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning, 2005.
[56] L. Rozenblit and F. Keil, The misunderstood limits of folk science: an illusion of explanatory depth, Cog. Sci., vol. 26, 2002.
[57] S.H. Schwartz and W. Bilsky, Toward A Universal Psychological Structure of Human Values, J. of Personality and Social Psych. vol. 53, no. 3, 1987.
[58] P. Shah and P. A. Carpenter, Conceptual Limitations in Comprehending Line Graphs, J. of Exp. Psych.: General, vol. 124, no. 1, 1995.
[59] P. Shah & E. Freedman, Bar and line graph comprehension: An interaction of top-down and bottom-up processes. Topics in Cog. Sci., 2009.
[60] P. Shah, R.E. Mayer & M. Hegarty, Graphs as aids to knowledge construction: Signaling techniques for guiding the process of graph comprehension. J. of Educ. Psych., vol 91, 1991.
[61] M. Siegrist, The use or misuse of three-dimensional graphs to represent lower-dimensional data, Behav. & Info. Tech, vol. 15, no. 2, 1996.
[62] C.S. Skinner, V.J. Strecher, and H. Hospers, Physicians' recommendations for mammography: do tailored messages make a difference? Amer. J. of Pub. Health, vol. 84, no. 1, 1994.
[63] D. Sprague & M. Tory, Motion in Casual InfoVis and the Interrelation between Personality, Performance, and Preference. Technical Report, 2009.
[64] Tableau, Tabeau Software Inc., http:/www.tableausoftware.com/.
[65] J. G. Trafton, S. B. Trickett, and F. E. Mintz, Connecting Internal and External Representations: Spatial Transformations of Scientific Visualizations. Foundations of Science, vol. 10 no. 1, 2005.
[66] E. R. Tufte, The Visual Display of Quantitative Information, 2nd ed. Graphics Press, 2001.
[67] H. Wainer, Picturing the Uncertain World: How to Understand, Communicate, and Control Uncertainty through Graphical Display. Princeton University Press, 2009.
[68] C. Ware, Visual Thinking: for Design, First Edition. Morgan Kaufmann, 2008.
[69] K. Yee and D. Fisher, Animated Exploration of Dynamic Graphs with Radial Layout, INFOVIS, 2001.
[70] J. Zacks, E. Levy, B. Tversky, and D. J. Schiano, Reading Bar Graphs: Effects of Extraneous Depth Cues and Graphical Context, J. of Exp. Psych.: Applied, vol. 4, no. 2, 1998.

Index Terms:
Desirable difficulites, cognitive efficiency, active processing, engagement, individual differences.
Citation:
Jessica Hullman, Eytan Adar, Priti Shah, "Benefitting InfoVis with Visual Difficulties," IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, vol. 17, no. 12, pp. 2213-2222, Dec. 2011, doi:10.1109/TVCG.2011.175
Usage of this product signifies your acceptance of the Terms of Use.