The Community for Technology Leaders
RSS Icon
Subscribe
Issue No.02 - April-June (2013 vol.4)
pp: 197-210
John E. Steephen , University of Hyderabad, Hyderabad
ABSTRACT
Affective adaptation is the process of weakening of the affective response of a constant or repeated affective stimulus by psychological processes. A modified exponentially weighted average computational model of affective adaptation, which predicts its time course and the resulting affective dynamics, is presented. In addition to capturing the primary features of affective adaptation, it is shown that the model is consistent with several previously reported characteristics of affective dynamics. For instance, the model shows that elicited emotion is determined by the position, displacement, velocity, and acceleration of the stimulus. It also demonstrates that affective after-reaction correlates positively with stimulus intensity and duration and that the duration-of-current-ownership, duration-of-prior-ownership, and time-elapsed-since-loss effects can be explained by it. The model exhibits the region-$(\beta)$ paradox that refers to the observation that stronger emotions sometimes abate faster than the weaker ones. The model also predicts that the proposed mechanisms underlying the paradox may have other effects on affective dynamics as well. Besides offering an explanation for the contradicting reports on emotion intensity-duration relationship, it is also proposed that adaptation processes activate quickly but deactivate slowly. Potential applications in affective computing as well as some new lines of empirical research are discussed.
INDEX TERMS
Adaptation models, Mathematical model, Computational modeling, Psychology, Differential equations, Predictive models, Appraisal, dynamical system, Affective adaptation, modeling human emotion, region-$(\beta)$ paradox, temporal dynamics
CITATION
John E. Steephen, "HED: A Computational Model of Affective Adaptation and Emotion Dynamics", IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, vol.4, no. 2, pp. 197-210, April-June 2013, doi:10.1109/T-AFFC.2013.2
REFERENCES
[1] M. Cabanac, "Physiological Role of Pleasure," Science, vol. 173, no. 4002, pp. 1103-1107, Sept. 1971.
[2] N.H. Frijda, Laws of Emotion. Erlbaum, 2007.
[3] P. Rozin, "Preadaptation and the Puzzles and Properties of Pleasure," Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, D. Kahneman, E. Diener, and N. Schwarz, eds., pp. 109-133, Russell Sage Foundation, 2003.
[4] A. Parducci, Happiness, Pleasure, and Judgment: The Contextual Theory and Its Applications. Erlbaum, 1995.
[5] C.A. Smith and R.S. Lazarus, "Emotion and Adaptation," Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, L.A. Pervin, ed., pp. 609-637, Guilford Press, 1990.
[6] P.C. Ellsworth and K.R. Scherer, "Appraisal Processes in Emotion," Handbook of Affective Sciences, R.J. Davidson and K.R. Scherer, eds., pp. 572-595, Oxford Univ. Press, 2003.
[7] J. Gratch and S. Marsella, "A Domain-Independent Framework for Modeling Emotion," Cognitive Systems Research, vol. 5, no. 4, pp. 269-306, 2004.
[8] R. Marinier and J. Laird, "Toward a Comprehensive Computational Model of Emotions and Feelings," Proc. Sixth Int'l Conf. Cognitive Modeling, pp. 172-177, 2004.
[9] S. Frederick and G. Loewenstein, "Hedonic Adaptation," Well-Being: The Foundations of Hedonic Psychology, D. Kahneman, E. Diener, and N. Schwarz, eds., pp. 302-329, Russell Sage Foundation, 2003.
[10] D.T. Gilbert, E. Driver-Linn, and T.D. Wilson, "The Trouble with Vronsky: Impact Bias in the Forecasting of Future Affective States," The Wisdom in Feeling: Psychological Processes in Emotional Intelligence, L.F. Barrett and P. Salovey, eds., pp. 114-143, Guilford Press, 2002.
[11] H. Helson, Adaptation-Level Theory. Harper & Row, 1964.
[12] S. Lyubomirsky, "Hedonic Adaptation to Positive and Negative Experiences," The Oxford Handbook of Stress, Health, and Coping, S. Folkman, ed., pp. 200-224, Oxford Univ. Press, 2010.
[13] T.D. Wilson and D.T. Gilbert, "Explaining Away: A Model of Affective Adaptation," Perspectives Psychological Sci., vol. 3, no. 5, pp. 370-386, 2008.
[14] E. Diener, C.R. Colvin, W.G. Pavot, and A. Allman, "The Psychic Costs of Intense Positive Affect," J. Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 61, no. 3, pp. 492-503, Sept. 1991.
[15] E. Aronson and J. Mills, "The Effect of Severity of Initiation on Liking for a Group," The J. Abnormal and Social Psychology, vol. 59, no. 2, pp. 177-181, 1959.
[16] P.G. Zimbardo, A.R. Cohen, M. Weisenberg, L. Dworkin, and I. Firestone, "Control of Pain Motivation by Cognitive Dissonance," Science, vol. 151, no. 3707, pp. 217-219, Jan. 1966.
[17] H.B. Gerard and G.C. Mathewson, "The Effects of Severity of Initiation on Liking for a Group: A Replication," J. Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 2, no. 3, pp. 278-287, 1966.
[18] S.E. Brandon and A.P. Silke, "Near and Long-Term Psychological Effects of Exposure to Terrorist Attacks," Psychology of Terrorism, B. Bongar, L.M. Brown, L.E. Beutler, J.N. Breckenridge, and P.G. Zimbardo, eds., pp. 175-193, Oxford Univ. Press, 2007.
[19] K.A. Rasinski, J. Berktold, T.W. Smith, and B.L. Albertson, America Recovers: A Follow-Up to a National Study of Public Response to the September 11th Terrorist Attacks. Univ. of Chicago Press, 2002.
[20] S.E. Taylor, "Asymmetrical Effects of Positive and Negative Events: The Mobilization-Minimization Hypothesis," Psychological Bull., vol. 110, no. 1, pp. 67-85, 1991.
[21] T.D. Wilson, D.T. Gilbert, and D.B. Centerbar, "Making Sense: The Causes of Emotional Evanescence," The Psychology of Economic Decisions: Rationality and Well-Being, I. Brocas and J.F. Carillo, eds., pp. 209-233, Oxford Univ. Press, 2002.
[22] D.T. Gilbert, M.D. Lieberman, C.K. Morewedge, and T.D. Wilson, "The Peculiar Longevity of Things Not So Bad," Psychological Science, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 14-19, 2004.
[23] N.H. Frijda, B. Mesquita, J. Sonnemans, and S. Van Goozen, "The Duration of Affective Phenomena or Emotions, Sentiments and Passions," Int'l Rev. Studies on Emotion, K.T. Strongman, ed., vol. 1, pp. 187-225, Wiley, 1991.
[24] J. Broekens, "Modeling the Experience of Emotion," Int'l J. Synthetic Emotions, vol. 1, no. 1, pp. 1-17, 2010.
[25] A. Parducci, "The Relativism of Absolute Judgments," Science Am., vol. 219, no. 6, pp. 84-90, 1968.
[26] B.G.S. Hardie, E.J. Johnson, and P.S. Fader, "Modeling Loss Aversion and Reference Dependence Effects on Brand Choice," Marketing Science, vol. 12, no. 4, pp. 378-394, Jan. 1993.
[27] H.E. Ryder and G.M. Heal, "Optimal Growth with Intertemporally Dependent Preferences," Rev. Economic Study, vol. 40, no. 1, pp. 1-31, 1973.
[28] J.G. March, "Variable Risk Preferences and Adaptive Aspirations," J. Economic Behavior and Organization, vol. 9, no. 1, pp. 5-24, 1988.
[29] D. Levinthal and J.G. March, "A Model of Adaptive Organizational Search," J. Economic Behavior and Organization, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 307-333, 1981.
[30] P. Brickman and D.T. Campbell, "Hedonic Relativism and Planning the Good Society," Adaptation-Level Theory: A Symp., M.H. Appley, ed., pp. 287-302, Academic Press, 1971.
[31] R. Port and T. Van Gelder, Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition. MIT Press, 1995.
[32] T. Bosse, M. Pontier, and J. Treur, "A Dynamical System Modelling Approach to Gross' Model of Emotion Regulation," Proc. Eighth Int'l Conf. Cognitive Modeling, vol. 7, pp. 187-192, 2007.
[33] N.T. Carnevale and M.L. Hines, The NEURON Book, first ed. Cambridge Univ Press, 2006.
[34] R.L. Solomon and J.D. Corbit, "An Opponent-Process Theory of Motivation: I. Temporal Dynamics of Affect," Psychological Rev., vol. 81, no. 2, pp. 119-145, Mar. 1974.
[35] R.L. Solomon, "The Opponent-Process Theory of Acquired Motivation: The Costs of Pleasure and the Benefits of Pain," Am. Psychological, vol. 35, no. 8, pp. 691-712, Aug. 1980.
[36] C.K. Hsee, P. Salovey, and R.P. Abelson, "The Quasi-Acceleration Relation: Satisfaction as a Function of the Change of Velocity of Outcome over Time," J. Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 30, pp. 96-111, 1994.
[37] C.K. Hsee and R.P. Abelson, "Velocity Relation: Satisfaction as a Function of the First Derivative of Outcome over Time," J. Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 60, no. 3, pp. 341-347, 1991.
[38] C.K. Hsee, R.P. Abelson, and P. Salovey, "The Relative Weighting of Position and Velocity in Satisfaction," Psychological Science, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 263-266, July 1991.
[39] R.E. Lucas, A.E. Clark, Y. Georgellis, and E. Diener, "Reexamining Adaptation and the Set Point Model of Happiness: Reactions to Changes in Marital Status," J. Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 84, no. 3, pp. 527-539, 2003.
[40] E. Diener, R.E. Lucas, and C.N. Scollon, "Beyond the Hedonic Treadmill: Revising the Adaptation Theory of Well-Being," Am. Psychology, vol. 61, no. 4, pp. 305-314, 2006.
[41] R.B. McCall, "Individual Differences in the Pattern of Habituation at 5 and 10 Months of Age," Developmental Psychology, vol. 15, no. 5, pp. 559-569, 1979.
[42] M.H. Bornstein and A.A. Benasich, "Infant Habituation: Assessments of Individual Differences and Short-Term Reliability at Five Months," Child Development, vol. 57, no. 1, pp. 87-99, Feb. 1986.
[43] S. Sirois and D. Mareschal, "Models of Habituation in Infancy," Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 6, no. 7, pp. 293-298, July 2002.
[44] R.F. Thompson and W.A. Spencer, "Habituation: A Model Phenomenon for the Study of Neuronal Substrates of Behavior," Psychological Rev., vol. 73, no. 1, pp. 16-43, 1966.
[45] P.M. Groves and R.F. Thompson, "Habituation: A Dual-Process Theory," Psychological Rev., vol. 77, no. 5, pp. 419-450, 1970.
[46] P.M. Groves, D. Lee, and R.F. Thompson, "Effects of Stimulus Frequency and Intensity on Habituation and Sensitization in Acute Spinal Cat," Physiology and Behavior, vol. 4, pp. 383-388, 1969.
[47] E. Diener, E. Sandvik, and W. Pavot, "Happiness Is the Frequency, Not the Intensity, of Positive versus Negative Affect," Assessing Well-Being: The Collected Works of Ed Diener, E. Diener, ed., pp. 213-231, Springer, 2009.
[48] N. Bolger, A. DeLongis, R.C. Kessler, and E.A. Schilling, "Effects of Daily Stress on Negative Mood," J. Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 57, no. 5, pp. 808-18, Nov. 1989.
[49] E. Suh, E. Diener, and F. Fujita, "Events and Subjective Well-Being: Only Recent Events Matter," J. Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 70, no. 5, pp. 1091-1102, May 1996.
[50] R.M. Church, V. LoLordo, J.B. Overmier, R.L. Solomon, and L.H. Turner, "Cardiac Responses to Shock in Curarized Dogs: Effects of Shock Intensity and Duration, Warning Signal, and Prior Experience with Shock," J. Comparative and Physiological Psychology, vol. 62, no. 1, pp. 1-7, 1966.
[51] G. Loewenstein and N. Sicherman, "Do Workers Prefer Increasing Wage Profiles?" J. Labor Economics, vol. 9, no. 1, p. 67, Jan. 1991.
[52] R. Frank and R.M. Hutchens, "Wages, Seniority, and the Demand for Rising Consumption Profiles," J. Economic Behavior and Organization, vol. 21, no. 3, pp. 251-276, 1993.
[53] R. Frank, "Frames of Reference and the Intertemporal Wage Profile," Choice over Time, first ed., G. Loewenstein and J. Elster, eds., Russell Sage Foundation, 1992.
[54] V. Quinsey, "Some Applications of Adaptation-Level Theory to Aversive Behavior," Psychological Bull., vol. 73, no. 6, pp. 441-450, 1970.
[55] M.A. Strahilevitz and G. Loewenstein, "The Effect of Ownership History on the Valuation of Objects," J. Consumer Research, vol. 25, no. 3, pp. 276-289, Dec. 1998.
[56] J. Sonnemans and N.H. Frijda, "The Structure of Subjective Emotional Intensity," Cognition Emotion, vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 329-350, 1994.
[57] J. Sonnemans and N.H. Frijda, "The Determinants of Subjective Emotional Intensity," Cognition Emotion, vol. 9, no. 5, pp. 483-506, 1995.
[58] B. Rimé, P. Philippot, S. Boca, and B. Mesquita, "Long-Lasting Cognitive and Social Consequences of Emotion: Social Sharing and Rumination," European Rev. Social Psychology, W. Stroebe and M. Hewstone, eds., vol. 3, pp. 225-258, Wiley, 1992.
[59] S. Marsella and J. Gratch, "A Step Toward Irrationality: Using Emotion to Change Belief," Proc. First Int'l Joint Conf. Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems, pp. 334-341, 2002.
[60] M. Lahnstein, "The Emotive Episode Is a Composition of Anticipatory and Reactive Evaluations," Proc. Symp. Agents that Want and Like, vol. 5, pp. 62-69, 2005.
[61] B.R. Steunebrink, M. Dastani, and J.C. Meyer, "A Formal Model of Emotions: Integrating Qualitative and Quantitative Aspects," Proc. Fifth Prestigious Applications of Intelligent Systems, pp. 256-260, 2008.
[62] A. Ortony, G.L. Clore, and A. Collins, The Cognitive Structure of Emotions. Cambridge Univ. Press, 1988.
[63] C. Becker, S. Kopp, and I. Wachsmuth, "Simulating the Emotion Dynamics of a Multimodal Conversational Agent," Proc. Int'l Conf. Affective Dialogue Systems, vol. 3068, pp. 154-165, 2004.
[64] J.D. Velásquez, "Modeling Emotion-Based Decision-Making," Proc. AAAI Fall Symp. Emotional and Intelligent: The Tangled Knot of Cognition, pp. 164-169, 1998.
[65] J. Broekens, W.A. Kosters, and F.J. Verbeek, "On Affect and Self-Adaptation: Potential Benefits of Valence-Controlled Action-Selection," Proc. Second Int'l Work-Conf. Interplay between Natural and Artificial Computation: Bio-Inspired Modeling of Cognitive Tasks, pp. 357-366, 2007.
[66] E. Hogewoning, J. Broekens, J. Eggermont, and E.G.P. Bovenkamp, "Strategies for Affect-Controlled Action-Selection in Soar-RL," Proc. Second Int'l Work-Conf. Nature Inspired Problem-Solving Methods in Knowledge Engineering: Interplay between Natural and Artificial Computation, vol. 4528, pp. 501-510, 2007.
[67] T. Bosse, R. Duell, Z.A. Memon, J. Treur, and C.N. Van Der Wal, "A Multi-Agent Model for Emotion Contagion Spirals Integrated within a Supporting Ambient Agent Model," Principles of Practice in Multi-Agent Systems, J.J. Yang, M. Yokoo, T. Ito, Z. Jin, and P. Scerri, eds. vol. 5925, pp. 48-67, Springer, 2009.
[68] T. Bosse and F.P.J. de Lange, "Development of Virtual Agents with a Theory of Emotion Regulation," Proc. IEEE/WIC/ACM Int'l Conf. Intelligent Agent Technology, pp. 461-468, 2008.
[69] J.J. Gross, "The Emerging Field of Emotion Regulation: An Integrative Review," Rev. General Psychology, vol. 2, no. 3, pp. 271-299, 1998.
[70] H. Miwa, T. Okuchi, K. Itoh, H. Takanobu, and A. Takanishi, "A New Mental Model for Humanoid Robots for Human Friendly Communication Introduction of Learning System, Mood Vector and Second Order Equations of Emotion," Proc. IEEE Int'l Conf. Robotics and Automation, vol. 3, pp. 3588-3593, 2003.
[71] J. Broekens and D. DeGroot, "Scalable and Flexible Appraisal Models for Virtual Agents," Proc. Int'l Conf. Computer Games, Artificial Intelligence Design and Education, pp. 208-215, 2004.
[72] S. Marsella and J. Gratch, "EMA: A Process Model of Appraisal Dynamics," Cognitive Systems Research, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 70-90, 2009.
[73] J. Broekens, D. Degroot, and W. Kosters, "Formal Models of Appraisal: Theory, Specification, and Computational Model," Cognitive Systems Research, vol. 9, no. 3, pp. 173-197, 2008.
[74] C. Bartneck, "Integrating the OCC Model of Emotions in Embodied Characters," Proc. Workshop Virtual Conversational Characters, pp. 39-48, 2002.
[75] J. Broekens, W.A. Kosters, and F.J. Verbeek, "Affect, Anticipation, and Adaptation: Affect-Controlled Selection of Anticipatory Simulation in Artificial Adaptive Agents," Adaptive Behavior, vol. 15, no. 4, pp. 397-422, 2007.
[76] E. Diener, R. Larsen, S. Levine, and R.A. Emmons, "Intensity and Frequency: Dimensions Underlying Positive and Negative Affect," J. Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 48, no. 5, pp. 1253-1265, 1985.
[77] D.T. Gilbert, E.C. Pinel, T.D. Wilson, S.J. Blumberg, and T.P. Wheatley, "Immune Neglect: A Source of Durability Bias in Affective Forecasting," J. Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 75, no. 3, pp. 617-638, Sept. 1998.
17 ms
(Ver 2.0)

Marketing Automation Platform Marketing Automation Tool